How is the Self Perception Profile for Adolescents different from the Self Perception Profile for Children?

34.How is the Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents different from the Self-Perception Profile forChildren?A.The Adolescent Profile includes sections on job competence, romantic appeal, and closefriendships.B.The Adolescent Profile does not generate a global self-worth score.C.The Child Profile has fewer mathematical and logical items.D.The Child Profile does not assess different domains of self.

Accessibility: Keyboard NavigationBlooms: AnalysisDifficulty: ModerateLearning Objective: 11-02 Explain self-esteem and self-concept.Santrock - Chapter 11 #3435.Children with high self-esteem

Get answer to your question and much more

Accessibility: Keyboard NavigationBlooms: KnowledgeDifficulty: ModerateLearning Objective: 11-02 Explain self-esteem and self-concept.Santrock - Chapter 11 #3536.In one study both boys and girls had high self-esteem in childhood. Which of the following statementsis correct?

Get answer to your question and much more

Accessibility: Keyboard NavigationBlooms: EvaluationDifficulty: ModerateLearning Objective: 11-02 Explain self-esteem and self-concept.Santrock - Chapter 11 #3637.In Coopersmith's research on boys' self-esteem (1967), all of the following parenting attributes wereassociated with boys' high self-esteem,except

Get answer to your question and much more

Accessibility: Keyboard NavigationBlooms: AnalysisDifficulty: BasicLearning Objective: 11-02 Explain self-esteem and self-concept.Santrock - Chapter 11 #3738.If a child receives empty praise, it is likely that the child will haveA.appropriate self-esteem.B.inflated self-esteem.C.deflated self-esteem.D.None of these.

Accessibility: Keyboard NavigationBlooms: ComprehensionDifficulty: ModerateLearning Objective: 11-02 Explain self-esteem and self-concept.Santrock - Chapter 11 #38

  • Journal List
  • J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
  • v.26(2); 2017 Jul
  • PMC5510942

J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017 Jul; 26(2): 119–124.

Published online 2017 Jul 1.

Language: English | French

Abstract

Objective

The Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPPC) is a commonly used measure of self-concept in children, but little research has examined its psychometric properties in children newly-diagnosed with chronic illness.

Method

Confirmatory factor analysis and examination of reliability and convergent and discriminant validity of the SPPC was conducted in 31 children newly-diagnosed with asthma, diabetes, epilepsy, food allergy, or juvenile arthritis.

Results

The unidimensionality of each domain of the SPPC was confirmed, internal reliability was robust (α=.83–.95), and inter-domain polychoric correlations ranged from weak to strong (ρ=.05–.85) Convergent validity was demonstrated with measures of global self-concept and domains of quality of life. The Global Self-worth domain showed discriminant validity between children with and without comorbid mental disorder.

Conclusions

Findings extend the psychometric properties of the SPPC as a valid and reliable scale in children newly-diagnosed with chronic illness.

Keywords: chronic disease, confirmatory factor analysis, measurement, self-concept, validity

Résumé

Objectif

Le Profil d’auto-perception pour enfants (PAPE) sert habituellement à mesurer le concept de soi des enfants, mais peu de recherche a examiné ses propriétés psychométriques chez les enfants ayant récemment reçu un diagnostic de maladie chronique.

Méthode

L’analyse factorielle confirmatoire ainsi que l’examen de la fiabilité et de la validité convergente et discriminante du PAPE ont été menés auprès de 31 enfants ayant récemment reçu un diagnostic d’asthme, de diabète, d’épilepsie, d’allergie alimentaire ou d’arthrite juvénile.

Résultats

L’unidimensionnalité de chaque domaine du PAPE a été confirmée, la fiabilité interne était robuste (α = 0,83 à 0,95), et les corrélations polychoriques inter-domaines allaient de faibles à fortes (ρ = 0,05 à 0,85). La validité convergente était démontrée par des mesures du concept de soi général et des domaines de qualité de vie. Le domaine général du concept de soi montrait une validité discriminante entre les enfants avec et sans un trouble mental comorbide.

Conclusions

Les résultats confirment que les propriétés psychométriques du PAPE en font une échelle valide et fiable pour les enfants ayant récemment reçu un diagnostic de maladie chronique.

Mots clés: maladie chronique, analyse factorielle confirmatoire, mesure, concept de soi, validité

The Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPPC) is one of the most commonly used scales to measure self-concept in children (Harter, 2012). This widespread use by researchers and clinicians can be attributable to its extensive psychometric evaluations in several child populations (Harter, 2012; Muris, Meesters, & Fijen, 2003). However, little research has been conducted in children with chronic illness such as diabetes and epilepsy. One study replicated the factor structure of the SPPC in children with spina bifida, finding that it demonstrated partial measurement invariance in comparison to healthy controls (Thill et al., 2003).

Research confirming the psychometric properties of the SPPC in children with chronic illness is important to ensure that hypothesized domains of self-concept are measured as intended, facilitating valid across-group comparisons and providing confidence to researchers and clinicians that inferences are unbiased (Gregorich, 2006). Because children with chronic illness represent a large proportion of the population (van der Lee, Mokkink, Grootenhuis, Heymans, & Offringa, 2007), inaccurate assessment of their self-concept could have implications for individual treatment decisions. This is especially pertinent early in the illness course when there may be uncertainty in prognosis, prescribing/titrating new treatments, and adapting to having a chronic illness relatively early in life (Ferro, Gorter, & Boyle, 2015). This study aimed to replicate the unidimensionality and examine the reliability and validity of the SPPC domains in a sample of children newly-diagnosed with a chronic illness. We hypothesized that the unidimensionality of the SPPC domains would be confirmed and that it would be able to discriminate between children with a chronic illness with and without a comorbid mental disorder.

Methods

Sample

Fifty-six children aged 6–16 years newly-diagnosed with one of asthma, diabetes, epilepsy, food allergy, or juvenile arthritis were recruited from outpatient clinics at two children’s tertiary centres in Canada. Analyses were restricted to children aged 11–16 years (n=31), as these children self-reported the SPPC. Children had a mean age of 13.5 (SD 1.6) years and 52% were male. The mean illness duration was 1.9 (SD 1.7) months. Most parents (74%) were in a partnered relationship, 74% graduated university/college, and 65% had household incomes of ≥$75,000. Parents and children completed telephone interviews and mail questionnaires at baseline and again six months later. All participants provided informed consent/assent. The study received ethical approval from all relevant research ethics boards.

Measures

The 36-item SPPC is a self-reported scale designed to assess children’s perceptions of their competence, or self-concept, in six different domains: Scholastic Competence, Social Acceptance, Athletic Competence, Physical Appearance, Behavioral Conduct, and, Global Self-worth (Harter, 2012). Each item contains one positive and one negative description of a specific skill. Respondents are asked to choose which statement best describes them and then select whether the chosen statement was “Really true for me” or “Sort of true for me”. Domain scores are the mean of the item scores with higher scores indicating more positive self-concept. The SPPC is validated for children aged 8–15 years and its initial development demonstrated strong psychometric properties (Harter, 2012).

Four items from the General Self-image domain of the Self-Description Questionnaire (SDQ) were used to assess child global self-concept (Ferro & Boyle, 2013b; Marsh, 1988). The items are rated using a five-point Likert scale, with scores indicating better self-concept. The scale has been validated by Statistics Canada and used in a number of epidemiological studies in Canada, including the National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth. It demonstrated adequate internal consistency in this sample (α=.82).

Child quality of life was measured using the self-reported KIDSCREEN-27 (Ravens-Sieberer et al., 2007). It measures quality of life across five domains: Physical Well-being, Psychological Well-being, Autonomy & Parent Relations, Social Support & Peers, and, School Environment. Item responses are based on a five-point Likert scale and T-scores for each domain are computed, whereby higher scores indicate better quality of life. It is valid and reliable in children aged 8–18 years (Ravens-Sieberer et al., 2007). Internal consistency for each domain from in this sample was adequate (α=.70–.90).

Mental disorder was measured using the child-reported and psychometrically robust Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID) to determine if children met DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for the following disorders: major depressive, separation anxiety, social or specific phobia, generalized anxiety, attention-deficit hyperactivity, oppositional defiant, or conduct (Sheehan et al., 2010).

Analysis

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA; maximum likelihood estimation) was used to determine the goodness-of-fit for the SPPC domains. Determination of model fit was based on five fit indices: χ2 goodness-of-fit, Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR), and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). Adequate fit was defined using the following cutoffs: χ2 p≥0.05, CFI≥0.90, TLI≥0.90, RMSEA≤ 0.08, and SRMR≤0.08 (Schreiber, Nora, Stage, Barlow, & King, 2006). Following previously published guidelines, if ≥3 fit indices met the specified threshold, fit was deemed adequate (Ferro & Boyle, 2013a, 2013b). Due to strong arguments against using the RMSEA in models with small degrees of freedom and small sample sizes in which the RMSEA can be artificially large, we include, but not consider it in decisions regarding model fit (Kenny, Kaniskan, & McCoach, 2015). Mean domain scores were compared over time and to normative data (n=577) using paired and independent samples t-tests, respectively. Convergent validity of the SPPC was examined using Spearman correlations with the SDQ and KIDSCREEN-27. Discriminant validity between children with and without comorbid mental disorder was examined using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test.

Monte Carlo simulations were used to ensure this study was adequately powered to estimate the unidimensionality of the SPPC domains (Wolf, Harrington, Clark, & Miller, 2013). In the first simulation (10,000 replications), we specified standardized factor loadings of .80. Under these conditions, statistical power was estimated to be, 1–β=.87. Then, we conducted CFA for each SPPC domain and used these values to estimate statistical power in subsequent simulations. With our data, statistical power was between .78 and .87. Having established that our exploratory study was adequately powered, we used bootstrapping (1,000 replications) to minimize potential bias associated with sampling error (Efron & Tibshirani, 1986). Data were analyzed using SAS 9.2 and Mplus 6.11.

Results

Each SPPC domain demonstrated adequate fit to the data according to the predefined criteria (Table 1). Factor loadings ranged from moderate (.40) to strong (.97). The SPPC demonstrated excellent internal consistency, with a low of α=.83 for Behavioral Conduct and a high of α=.95 for Athletic Competence.

Table 1

Bootstrapped factor solution for the Self-Perception Profile for Children

Factor (α reliability)/ItemBaseline

Factor LoadingResidual VarianceR2
Scholastic Competence (.88): χ2=15.02, CFI=0.930, TLI=0.841, RMSEA=0.149 [0.104, 0.185], SRMR=0.072
1. Some kids feel that they are very good at their school work .73 .47 .53
7. Some kids feel like they are just as smart as other kids their age .87 .25 .75
13. Some kids are pretty slow in finishing their school work .62 .62 .38
19. Some kids often forget what they learn .40 .39 .16
25. Some kids do very well at their classwork .78 .40 .60
31. Some kids have trouble figuring out the answers in school .84 .29 .71
Social Competence (.91): χ2=10.76, CFI=0.983, TLI=0.903, RMSEA=0.081 [0.000, 0.128], SRMR=0.046
2. Some kids find it hard to make friends .74 .45 .55
8. Some kids know how to make classmates like them .83 .32 .68
14. Some kids don’t have the social skills to make friends .97 .06 .94
20. Some kids understand how to get peers to accept them .76 .42 .58
26. Some kids wish they knew how to make more friends .69 .67 .48
32. Some kids know how to become popular .72 .48 .52
Athletic Competence (.95): χ2=10.32, CFI=0.991, TLI=0.936, RMSEA=0.070 [0.000, 0.120], SRMR=0.024
3. Some kids do very well at all kinds of sports .92 .15 .85
9. Some kids wish they could be a lot better at sports .87 .24 .76
15. Some kids think they could do well at just about any new sports activity they haven’t tried .87 .24 .76
before
21. Some kids feel that they are better than others their age at sports .87 .24 .76
27. In games and sports some kids usually watch instead of play .89 .30 .79
33. Some kids don’t do well at new outdoor games .92 .15 .85
Physical Appearance (.92): χ2=10.80, CFI=0.983, TLI=0.907, RMSEA=0.082 [0.000, 0.129], SRMR=0.046
4. Some kids are happy with the way they look .92 .16 .85
10. Some kids are happy with their height and weight .61 .44 .39
16. Some kids wish their body was different .79 .38 .63
22. Some kids wish their physical appearance (how they look) was different .77 .40 .60
28. Some kids wish something about their face or hair looked different .84 .30 .70
34. Some kids think that they are good looking .82 .34 .67
Behavioral Conduct (.83): χ2=11.45, CFI=0.942, TLI=0.877, RMSEA=0.095 [0.000, 0.139], SRMR=0.066
5. Some kids often do not like the way they behave .62 .62 .38
11. Some kids usually do the right thing .79 .37 .63
17. Some kids usually act the way they know they are supposed to .81 .35 .65
23. Some kids usually get in trouble because of things they do .55 .70 .30
29. Some kids do things they know they shouldn’t do .67 .56 .44
35. Some kids behave themselves very well .58 .27 .34
Global Self-worth (.85): χ2=11.50, CFI=0.960, TLI=0.847, RMSEA=0.096 [0.000, 0.140], SRMR=0.055
6. Some kids are often unhappy with themselves .71 .50 .50
12. Some kids don’t like the way they are leading their life .63 .60 .40
18. Some kids are happy with themselves as a person .92 .15 .85
24. Some kids like the kind of person they are .56 .69 .31
30. Some kids are very happy being the way they are .80 .36 .64
36. Some kids are not very happy with the way they do a lot of things .68 .33 .46

Mean scores for each SPPC domain are shown in Table 2. There were no significant differences between baseline and six-month SPPC scores. Only Behavioral Conduct was found to be significantly different compared to normative controls (3.3 vs. 3.0; p=.007). Intra-domain polychoric correlations over time were strong, .62–.94 (Table 3).

Table 2

Comparisons of Self-concept

BaselineSix MonthsPopulation NormsBaseline vs. Six MonthsBaseline vs. NormsSix Months vs. Norms
Scholastic competence 3.1 (0.7) 3.1 (0.7) 3.0 (0.6) −0.59 (.562) 0.89 (.371) 0.90 (.371)
Social competence 2.9 (0.8) 3.1 (0.7) 3.1 (0.7) −1.35 (.189) −1.54 (.125) 0.00 (.999)
Athletic competence 2.7 (0.9) 2.9 (0.9) 3.0 (0.7) −1.13 (.268) −1.83 (.077) −0.76 (.446)
Physical appearance 3.0 (0.8) 2.9 (0.8) 3.0 (0.7) 0.18 (.862) 0.00 (.999) −0.77 (.442)
Behavioral conduct 3.3 (0.6) 3.3 (0.6) 3.0 (0.6) −0.07 (.946) 2.72 (.007) 2.72 (.007)
Global self-worth 3.2 (0.6) 3.2 (0.8) 3.2 (0.6) −0.29 (.773) 0.00 (.999) 0.00 (.999)

Table 3

Polychoric correlation matrix of factors from the self-perception profile for children

Six months

BaselineScholastic competenceSocial competenceAthletic competencePhysical appearanceBehavioural conductGlobal self-worth
Scholastic competence .62*** .25 .28 .42* .26 .24
Social competence .35* .75*** .62*** .75*** .40* .72***
Athletic competence .37* .24 .94*** .69*** .28 .75***
Physical appearance .53*** .49*** .56*** .86*** .43** .79***
Behavioral conduct .22 .44** .03 .05 .70*** .59***
Global self-worth .59*** .69*** .46** .88*** .37* .72***

Convergent validity of the SPPC was demonstrated with medium-sized correlations with related measures. SDQ: General Self-image—SPPC Global Self-worth (ρ=.73; p<.001); KIDSCREEN-27: School Environment—SPPC Scholastic Competence (ρ=0.36; p=.048), Social Support & Peers—SPPC Social Competence (ρ=.48; p=.007), Physical Well-being—SPPC Athletic Competence (ρ=.30; p=0.097); Physical Well-being—SPPC Physical Appearance (ρ=.26; p=.166), and Psychological Well-being—SPPC Behavioral Conduct (ρ=.56; p=.001). The Global Self-worth domain discriminated between children with chronic illness and comorbid mental disorders (n=6) and those with a chronic condition only (n=25), z=−2.78; p=.005.

Discussion

Findings replicated the unidimensionality of the SPPC domains and provided evidence supporting their internal reliability and convergent and discriminant validity in children with chronic illness. The magnitudes of parameter estimates and domain correlations were similar to other reports that sampled children in the general population (Harter, 2012; Muris et al., 2003) and those with chronic illnesses (Thill et al., 2003; Veerman, ten Brink, Straathof, & Treffers, 1996).

Despite evidence suggesting children with chronic illness have lower self-concept compared to healthy controls (Ferro & Boyle, 2013c, 2015), findings showed no significant declines compared to population norms. Indeed, our sample had higher scores for Behavioral Conduct. It has been suggested that such absence of differences may be due to different social reference groups; conscious distortion; unconscious denial; confusion of the real and ideal self; or, healthy adjustment to self-standards (Harter, 1999). Exaggerated preconceived notions of the negative experience of having a chronic illness may have framed children’s references in evaluating their own self-concepts at baseline. This frame of reference may have continued during the six-month follow-up, resulting in scores similar to population norms.

Our findings have three relevant limitations: 1) Our sample size was small; 2) We were not able to account for interdomain correlations in the CFA. This limitation is tempered given that our CFA parameter estimates were similar to previous reports. Because domains scores are based on mean item scores, comparisons across samples are unaffected; and, 3) Similar to previous samples validating the SPPC, our sample included generally affluent Caucasian families.

Conclusion

Findings extend the psychometric properties of the SPPC as a valid and reliable measure of self-concept in children newly-diagnosed with chronic illnesses. Replication of these findings is encouraged in larger samples of children with chronic illness.

Acknowledgements/Conflicts of Interest

The authors gratefully acknowledge the children, parents, and health professionals and their staff without whose participation, this study would not have been possible. This work was supported by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP-133645) to Dr. Ferro. During the conduct of this work, Dr. Ferro was supported by the Research Early Career Award from Hamilton Health Sciences. Dr. Ferro holds a Canada Research Chair (Tier 2) in Youth Mental Health.

References

  • Efron B, Tibshirani R. Bootstrap methods for standard errors, confidence intervals, and other measures of statistical accuracy. Stat Sci. 1986;1(1):54–77. [Google Scholar]
  • Ferro MA, Boyle MH. Brief report: testing measurement invariance and differences in self-concept between adolescents with and without physical illness or developmental disability. J Adolesc. 2013a;36(5):947–951. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ferro MA, Boyle MH. Longitudinal invariance of measurement and structure of global self-concept: a population-based study examining trajectories among adolescents with and without chronic illness. J Pediatr Psychol. 2013b;38(4):425–437. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ferro MA, Boyle MH. Self-concept among children and adolescents with a chronic illness: a meta-analytic review. Health Psychol. 2013c;32(8):839–848. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ferro MA, Boyle MH. The impact of chronic physical illness, maternal depressive symptoms, family functioning, and self-esteem on symptoms of anxiety and depression in children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2015;43(1):177–187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ferro MA, Gorter JW, Boyle MH. Trajectories of depressive symptoms during the transition to young adulthood: the role of chronic illness. J Affect Disord. 2015;174(2015):594–601. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gregorich SE. Do self-report instruments allow meaningful comparisons across diverse population groups? Testing measurement invariance using the confirmatory factor analysis framework. Med Care. 2006;44(11 Suppl 3):S78–94. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Harter S. The construction of the self A developmental perspective. New York: The Guildford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • Harter S. Self-Perception Profile for Children: Manual and Questionnaires (Grades 3–8) Denver: University of Denver; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • Kenny DA, Kaniskan B, McCoach DB. The performance of RMSEA in models with small degrees of freedom. Sociological Methods & Research. 2015;44(3):486–507. [Google Scholar]
  • Marsh HW. Self-Description Questionnaire-I. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • Muris P, Meesters C, Fijen P. The Self-Perception Profile for Children: further evidence for its factor structure, reliability, and validity. Person Individ Diff. 2003;35(8):1791–1802. [Google Scholar]
  • Ravens-Sieberer U, Auquier P, Erhart M, Gosch A, Rajmil L, Bruil J, …European KG. The KIDSCREEN-27 quality of life measure for children and adolescents: psychometric results from a cross-cultural survey in 13 European countries. Qual Life Res. 2007;16(8):1347–1356. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schreiber JB, Nora A, Stage FK, Barlow EA, King J. Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: a review. J Educ Res. 2006;99(6):323–337. [Google Scholar]
  • Sheehan DV, Sheehan KH, Shytle RD, Janavs J, Bannon Y, Rogers JE, …Wilkinson B. Reliability and validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID) J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71(3):313–326. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Thill AD, Holmbeck GN, Bryant FB, Nelson C, Skocic A, Uli N. Assessing the factorial invariance of Harter’s self-concept measures: comparing preadolescents with and without spina bifida using child, parent, and teacher report. J Pers Assess. 2003;81(2):111–122. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • van der Lee JH, Mokkink LB, Grootenhuis MA, Heymans HS, Offringa M. Definitions and measurement of chronic health conditions in childhood: a systematic review. JAMA. 2007;297(24):2741–2751. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Veerman JW, ten Brink LT, Straathof MA, Treffers PD. Measuring children’s self-concept with a Dutch version of the “self-perception profile for children”: factorial validity and invariance across a nonclinic and a clinic group. J Pers Assess. 1996;67(1):142–154. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wolf EJ, Harrington KM, Clark SL, Miller MW. Sample size requirements for structural equation models: an evaluation of power, bias, and solution propriety. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2013;73(6):913–934. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]


Articles from Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry


How is the self

How is the Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents different from the Self-Perception Profile for Children? The Adolescent Profile includes sections on job competence, romantic appeal, and close friendships. Harter's (1985) Self-Perception Profile for Children is used to evaluate: self-concept.

What is the self

The Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPPC) is a self-report magnitude estimation scale that measures a school-age child's sense of general self-worth and self-competence in the domain of academic skills (Harter 1982, 1985).

What differences exist between the self understanding of adolescence and that of emerging adults?

What differences exist between the self-understanding of adolescents and that of emerging adults? In emerging adulthood, self-understanding becomes more integrated. physical appearance. Erikson believed that _____ was the central issue for adolescents.

What happens to your self esteem during adolescence?

Self-esteem levels tend to decrease in early adolescence and increase in later adolescence (Baldwin and Hoffmann 2002), but those who have lower levels of self-esteem than others at one time point are likely to have lower self-esteem than others at the following time point as well (Robins and Trzesniewski 2005).